Learning doesn’t imprint itself into genes. Instead, natural selection favours genetic propensities to learn certain things. After generations of such selection, evolved descendants learn so fast that the behaviour has become ‘instinctive’.
Biological Inheritance and Behavioural Choice
In considering the contribution of biological inheritance to behavioural choice, it is useful to begin by examining organisms without neurological systems — the biological substrate of behavioural potential in metazoa (all animals except sponges). The biological potential of a plant, for example, is expressed through bodily structures that interact with the environment in specific ways. Plants have sensors that detect differences, such as the direction of light, and processes that follow from detection, like the re-orientation of leaves or the opening of flowers. These interactions with the external world mirror the internal developmental chain reactions that occur during the organism’s growth.
Similarly, the biological potential of animals, such as humans, is expressed through bodily structures (anatomy) and their associated interactive functions (physiology). In the case of neurological systems, humans inherit general anatomical structures with their potential interactive functions, such as species-specific sensory and motor abilities, and the categorising abilities for processing sensory and motor experiences. It’s important to note that this expression of biological potential does not include “innate” categories of the perceivable world that are “hard-wired” into the brain. This would imply the inheritance of acquired characteristics (Lamarckism), in which the categorising experiences of previous generations would have to be coded into the species’ genome.
The ontogenesis of behavioural potential in individuals is a personal evolution that depends on experiences from outside the body. However, the categorisation processes that guide this evolution are shaped by value-imposing systems that are phenotypic expressions of biological potential. These systems not only influence how behavioural potential adapts over a lifetime but also affect the probabilities of behavioural choices in specific situations. Value-imposing systems link neurological activity to the body’s homeostatic systems, so that in times of internal disequilibrium — such as hunger or fear — neurological events that restore equilibrium to the body are more likely than those that do not, all else being equal. Thus, the relationship between genes and specific behavioural choices is probabilistic and contingent on various factors.
Footnotes:
-
Bronowski, in The Ascent of Man (ep. 1: 47:00), distinguishes cultural adaptation (behaviours that can change) from biological adaptation (behaviours that cannot change).
-
For neurological givens, see Sacks (1995: 105).
-
Cf. the Baldwin Effect. Dawkins (2004: 325): Learning doesn’t imprint itself into genes. Instead, natural selection favours genetic propensities to learn certain things. After generations of such selection, evolved descendants learn so fast that the behaviour has become ‘instinctive’.
-
While types of somatic systems indicate what type of environment a lineage has adapted to, this is not the same as saying that categories of the environment exist in the brain of the individual before they begin to experience that environment.